StudySmarter - The all-in-one study app.
4.8 • +11k Ratings
More than 3 Million Downloads
Free
Attachment is a core aspect of growing up. A baby is naturally attached to their parents, which is beneficial for multiple reasons. Namely, the parent then raises their child and ensures their survival. Psychology investigated the attachment types and analysed the explanations behind these attachments. Their research has revealed that attachment does differ for babies and children.
So, what are these explanations and types of attachment? This article explains attachment that focuses on the learning theory, Bowlby’s Monotropic Theory, and Ainsworth’s Strange Situation. This article will outline and evaluate one or more explanations of attachment and examine how they work.
Attachment, Flaticon
Having previously looked at how attachments develop, we can now examine why they develop. There are three primary explanations for attachment that we will cover: Bowlby’s monotropic theory, the learning theory, and Ainsworth’s Strange Situation.
Learning theory explains attachment between a baby and their primary caregiver in terms of conditioning, which you may have encountered when studying behaviourism. It was dominant in the first half of the 20th century as a more popular explanation.
Bowlby’s monotropic theory takes a different approach, explaining attachment as an innate biological phenomenon, which is genetically programmed.
The strange situation study is an experiment conducted in the early 1970s that aimed to investigate whether or not there were different types of attachment in children when exposed to different scenarios with their caregivers and strangers. Mary Ainsworth originally conducted the study. She was an award-winning American-Canadian psychologist who specialised in attachment psychology.
Learning theory is a behaviourist theory based on classical and operant conditioning, which Ivan Pavlov and BF Skinner developed. Learning theory assumes that infants bond when they associate their mother (or another primary caregiver) with receiving food.
Receiving food is a pleasurable experience for the infant. When the caregiver (the neutral stimulus) is present during the feeding, the infant begins to associate that pleasant feeling with their caregiver. The child then bonds with their parent, which is an example of classical conditioning.
The child will seek out the caregiver to get food and other essentials, such as a diaper change or comfort. They associate the caregiver with the positive feelings they experience when the caregiver meets their needs. The more consistently the caregiver responds to the child, the more they reinforce the attachment, called operant conditioning.
In classical conditioning, first demonstrated in the famous Pavlov’s Dogs experiment, an individual begins to associate a neutral stimulus (something not associated with anything else) with an unconditioned response. There are a few important abbreviations we’ll need to define going forward. These are:
UCR – Unconditioned Response: when the response to a stimulus is not a result of conditioning (e.g., expressing pain when you fall).
UCS – Unconditioned Stimulus: when the stimulus is not associated with anything. (e.g., a plain piece of paper is unlikely to be associated with any feelings or behaviour).
CR – Conditioned Response: when a response is associated with a particular stimulus (e.g., if a dog bites you, you associate dogs with fear).
CS – Conditioned Stimulus: when a stimulus is associated with a response (e.g., the dog in the above example).
In classical conditioning, a person simultaneously experiences a stimulus as another stimulus that elicits a response. As a result, the individual associates the unconditioned stimulus with the response of the other stimulus. Below you can see how this can happen in infants when they begin to associate the positive feeling (UCR) they associate with receiving food (UCS) or their mother (CS), resulting in attachment.
Before learning: Food (UCS) → Pleasure (UCR)
During learning: Food (UCS) + Caregiver (CS) → Pleasure (UCR)
After learning: Caregiver (CS) → Pleasure (CR)
Operant conditioning is the idea that some conditioning occurs through reinforcement, which can be positive or negative.
If someone receives a reward after performing a behaviour, they are more likely to repeat that behaviour later, referred to as positive reinforcement.
Negative reinforcement is when the reward for performing the behaviour is the removal of an unpleasant stimulus.
For example, if a child loses recess time as a punishment for not doing their homework, they are likely to do their homework next time to avoid the unpleasant consequences.
Also, attachment occurs when children cry out for their caregiver when they need something.
For example, when a child cries and the caregiver feeds them, the unpleasant feeling of hunger goes away, which reinforces the behaviour of crying when hungry.
Dollard & Miller (1950) took this idea further and suggested that drive reduction causes attachment. Innate needs such as food and comfort are thus ‘drives’. When a child’s needs are met, we talk about drive reduction, a reward for the child. Dollard and Miller describe two types of reinforcers: primary (the object of the drive, e.g., food) and secondary (the provider of the drive, e.g., the mother who feeds the child).
The learning theory of attachment has its limitations, and we will outline and evaluate them in the following sections.
This theory is a behaviourist theory. Behaviourist theories usually lack validity because they represent an oversimplified version of human behaviour and rely heavily on the idea that attachment behaviour involves innate predispositions.
Learning theory places great emphasis on the role of food in attachment. Some studies have suggested that this may not be the case, for example:
Harlow (1958) found that baby rhesus monkeys were much more likely to cling to a rag ‘mother’ doll (which offered no food) than to a wire doll that provided food. Harlow thus suggests that it may be the comfort rather than food that drives attachment.
Similarly, Lorenz (1935) found that baby greylag geese clung to the first moving subject they saw after hatching. Before the first feeding, it was an almost instantaneous attachment, suggesting that food was not the driving force for attachment.
Another attachment theory is John Bowlby’s monotropic theory which he created between 1969 and 1988. Influenced by psychodynamics and evolutionary psychology, the theory suggests attachment is evolutionary and essential for survival. Bowlby explains that attachment behaviours of both infants and caregivers have evolved through natural selection and serve the evolutionary purpose of ensuring infant survival.
Through his research, Bowlby defined a critical period during which babies must bond with a caregiver. Otherwise, there is a risk that they will not bond at all later in life. According to Bowlby, this carries detrimental consequences.
This critical period is between birth and 2.5/3 years. In later revisions, Bowlby added that there might be a sensitive period, between 0 and 5 years after the critical period, where the attachment can form. However, it takes longer and requires more effort.
According to Bowlby, monotropy is a child’s innate biological need to bond with one primary caregiver. This relationship is considered the most important in the child’s life, survival, and psychological well-being. While other attachments can be made, this primary attachment is the most significant.
This attachment forms through social releasers innate behaviours such as crying, eye contact, clinging and smiling. These behaviours are biologically programmed to encourage the parent to bond with the child and respond to their needs. The child’s ability to perform these behaviours and the parent’s ability to pick up on them are both considered evolutionary advantages and therefore have become innate through natural selection.
We can see the internal working model as a ‘blueprint’ that a child learns for navigating social relationships. It also represents what the relationships they form throughout life might look like. The child learns this from the first, monotropic attachment with their primary caregiver, influencing their future platonic and romantic relationships.
The model involves three main features:
A model for the trustworthiness of others – if their primary caregiver has been consistently responsive to their needs, the child is more likely to perceive others as trustworthy.
A model of the self as valuable – if the child is treated with respect, and if their needs are met, they will see themselves as valuable.
A model of the self as effective in interacting with others – if the child’s social releasers are adequately responded to, the child will see themselves as effective in interacting with others.
Lorenz’s study of greylag geese mentioned earlier supports Bowlby’s explanation of attachment (monotropic theory), as it suggests attachment is an innate, biological function. However, since this is an animal study, it is hard to generalise the findings to humans.
Rutter’s Romanian Orphan Study (1998) discovered that the earlier the children in the study were adopted, the faster they progressed in their development. This finding supports Bowlby’s idea that attachments can affect development later in life and also supports the idea that it is more difficult to form these attachments as a child gets older.
Like the learning theory, this theory is also limited by its overemphasis on purely biological drives such as food. Harlow (1959) points out that comfort is an essential factor in attachment, which Bowlby’s theory does not consider.
Rutter’s (2007) later research of Romanian orphanages found no significant difference in attachment for children under or over six months of age. Age thus may not be such a substantial factor in a child’s ability to attach.
Another study by Schaffer and Emerson (1964) studied 60 Scottish infants in the first 18 months of life. It found that infants were more likely to bond with caregivers who responded accurately and consistently to their signals, supporting Bowlby’s idea of social releasers.
However, they also found that babies can develop multiple strong attachments by ten months of age, challenging Bowlby’s idea that they only have one.
Bowlby’s theory tends to take an ‘all or nothing’ approach to attachment. However, a study by Mary Ainsworth (1970) investigated different attachment qualities and found three different attachment types with other developmental consequences. The study points out the limitations of Bowlby’s theory as it shows there is much more to attachment than he assumed.
Ainsworth attempted to assess how children would react when exposed to a new situation, precisely, how they would feel and behave when their primary caregiver (in this case, their mother) left them. They were then left in the presence of a stranger.
The study was conducted on 12 to 18-month-old children, and researchers carried out eight different scenarios. Ainsworth defined different behaviours that were measured to determine the children’s attachment types:
The children’s behaviour was recorded using a controlled observation to measure the attachment. This process was comprised of consecutive sections, each lasting approximately 3 minutes. These are as follows;
Mary Ainsworth concluded that children can have three distinct attachment types with their primary caregiver (secure attachment, insecure-resistant attachment, and insecure-avoidant attachment).
15% were insecure-avoidant, 70% were securely attached, and 15% were insecure-resistant.
This challenged the previously accepted idea that attachment was something a child either had or didn’t have, as theorised by Ainsworth’s colleague John Bowlby. Parents play the most significant role in an infant’s life. They teach them all their basic skills, including forming healthy attachments.
Issues exist because the study lacked ecological validity (being conducted in a laboratory setting), and behaviours of both parent and child could reflect this (they may act differently, and the child can pick up on these subtle cues). However, the study is reliable as it did account for different variables.
It is somewhat innate, as to ensure survival, attachment needs to occur at a consistent level.
Many researchers have established various stages of attachment. Schaffer and Emerson in a longitudinal study established the following stages: pre attachment, indiscriminate attachment, discriminate attachment, and multiple attachments.
The main theories are the learning theory of attachment (involving operant and classical conditioning) and Bowlby's monotropic attachment theory.
Attachment theory is how an individual forms a bond with another. The bond is shared, and research studies this phenomenon by analysing influences on these relationships.
Research has shown that when an attachment is disrupted for a child, it affects later childhood and adulthood. It can impact self-esteem, self-reliance, and those who suffered as children (from abuse, for instance) can develop disorders such as post-traumatic stress disorder.
Be perfectly prepared on time with an individual plan.
Test your knowledge with gamified quizzes.
Create and find flashcards in record time.
Create beautiful notes faster than ever before.
Have all your study materials in one place.
Upload unlimited documents and save them online.
Identify your study strength and weaknesses.
Set individual study goals and earn points reaching them.
Stop procrastinating with our study reminders.
Earn points, unlock badges and level up while studying.
Create flashcards in notes completely automatically.
Create the most beautiful study materials using our templates.
Sign up to highlight and take notes. It’s 100% free.